Studies have long indicated that effort increases focus on the attentional target and increases distraction inhibition and this type of cognitive control enhances performance (Kahneman, 1973). However, more evidence also shows that a reduction in effortful control can also improve performance, such as in creativity, implicit learning and sensorimotor skills, consistent with the multi-action plan (MAP) model in sport performance (Beilock et al., 2002; Bortoli et al., 2012; Ding et al., 2014a; Stillman et al., 2014; Bertollo et al., 2015, 2016; Amer et al., 2016). We use one form of mindfulness meditation – integrative body-mind training (IBMT) in our series of randomized studies. IBMT emphasizes no effort or less effort to control mind and opening awareness to internal and external stimuli with an attitude of acceptance and equanimity. Our results show that as few as 5 sessions of IBMT (20–30 min per session) can improve attention, positive emotion and diverse cognitive performance including creativity, working memory, conflict resolution and learning (Tang et al., 2007, 2014, 2015; Posner et al., 2010; Ding et al., 2014a,b; Fan et al., 2014, 2015; Tang, 2017). This raises the possibility that less effortful attention or effortless attention can contribute to performance in activities involving creativity, sensorimotor skills or implicit learning.
Based on recent findings, we propose a framework for a relationship among attention, effort and optimal performance, as shown in Figure 1. Optimal performance often refers to an effortless and automatic, flow-like state of performance. Mindfulness (mindful acceptance) regulates the focus of attention to optimal focus (balanced attention) on the core component of the action, avoiding too much attention that could be detrimental for elite performance (Bertollo et al., 2016). Balanced attention is a trained state that can optimize any particular attentional activity on the dual-process spectrum. One can exert minimal effort to maintain balanced attention, resulting in a large impact on performance in cognition, positive emotion, health and quality of life. To optimize tasks that require high effort and explicit processing such as working memory, one can reallocate attentional resources, resulting in more efficiently focused attention and less effort. To optimize tasks that require low effort and implicit processing such as creativity or sensorimotor skills, one can bring diffused attention to the task, resulting in more control and monitoring. Through balanced attention, different activities with different cognitive demands can be optimized with a balance of implicit and explicit processing, the appropriate level of attention and effort. Balanced attention has also been called the “being” state (Tang and Posner, 2009, 2014; Tang et al., 2015; Tang, 2017).
Figure 1. Attention, effort, and optimal performance. Balanced attention improves performance in both high effort, explicit processing tasks and low-effort, implicit processing tasks.
What are the underlying mechanisms supporting these distinct processes? Neuroimaging research has suggested that explicit processing with more effort, such as working memory tasks, often recruits the frontoparietal network (Takeuchi et al., 2010; Tang and Posner, 2009, 2014; Ekman et al., 2016; Nissim et al., 2017). The frontoparietal network mainly includes the lateral frontal and parietal cortex and supports continuous effort. It should be noted that it is impossible to maintain a steady and continuous effort because attention states are in constant fluctuation regardless of ongoing task demands (Petersen and Posner, 2012; Tang, 2017; Tang et al., 2017). Studies have shown that attentional lapses lead to poor performance on the task and are associated with midline frontal areas such as anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Adam et al., 2015; Chang et al., 2015). In contrast, implicit processing with less effort, such as creativity and sensorimotor tasks, also involves the ACC, insula and striatum (Tang and Posner, 2009, 2014; Ding et al., 2014a,b). The ACC is involved in monitoring and maintaining a state by reducing conflict with other states; the insula involves switching between states; and the striatum is associated with the reward experience and habitual responses to make state maintenance easier (Tang et al., 2012, 2015; Tang and Tang, 2013). Meanwhile, the ACC and insula also collaborate to support the role of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) in maintaining the effortless state, which has parasympathetic dominance indexed by lower skin conductance response (SCR) and greater high frequency heart rate variability (HRV) (Tang et al., 2009; Tang, 2017). In contrast, sympathetic dominance more often occurs in effortful processing that requires alertness and activation of the frontoparietal network (Tang et al., 2012, 2009; Tang and Posner, 2014). These findings are consistent with the results of optimal and suboptimal performance in sports (Bertollo et al., 2013, 2015).
Studies have elucidated the interaction and dynamics between the central nervous system (CNS) and ANS (Critchley et al., 2003; Tang and Posner, 2009; Tang et al., 2009; Critchley and Harrison, 2013; Tang, 2017). For example, we examined the brain and physiological changes at rest before, during, and after 5 sessions of IBMT and active control—relaxation training. During and after training, compared to the relaxation control, the IBMT group showed significantly greater parasympathetic activity in each of these measures including heart rate, respiratory amplitude and rate, HRV and SCR. During and after IBMT, differences in HRV and EEG power suggested greater involvement of the ANS. Imaging data showed greater ACC, striatum and insula activity in the IBMT group. Most importantly, frontal midline ACC theta was also correlated with high-frequency HRV, suggesting control by the ACC over parasympathetic activity (Tang et al., 2009; Tang, 2017). These results indicate that brief IBMT induces better regulation of the ANS by a midline ACC brain system, suggesting the interaction and coordination of body and mind following IBMT, a form of mindfulness that optimizes activities for maximal self-control, attention and efficiency with minimal effort (Tang, 2017). Other studies have shown that parasympathetic activity is associated with the flow state (de Manzano et al., 2010; Keller et al., 2011; Thomson and Jaque, 2011; Jacobs, 2014), a prime example of balanced attention in which high control is achieved with low subjective mental effort (Bruya, 2010). We call this mechanism “parasympathetic-attentional interaction” or “PA mind-body interaction.”
In summary, growing empirical evidence indicates that PA mind-body interaction often triggers optimal performance and is one possible mechanism for optimizing performance (Tang and Posner, 2009, 2014; Bruya, 2010; Tang et al., 2012; Tang, 2017). PA mind-body interaction can also have a large impact on positive emotion, health benefits, quality of life and self-growth. The field of body-mind practice is rapidly growing. However the majority of research focuses on health and behavior effects (and related brain changes) from training (Lutz et al., 2008; Tang et al., 2015). There has been less effort to scientifically investigate the underlying mechanisms (e.g., key biomarkers) of mind-body interaction and optimal performance when practitioners engage and maintain an effortless state. The current perspective aims to address this research gap. By integrating evidence from neuroimaging with evidence from physiology we propose the key brain markers in the ACC-insula-striatum network and the key physiological markers in the parasympathetic regulation of HRV and SCR. This effort will also shed light on how humans learn and practice physical and mental training effectively. Future studies can examine the relationship between PA mind-body interaction and short-term or long-term training such as mindfulness and its underlying mechanisms, using psychosocial, physiological, multimodal neuroimaging, and genetic methods.
All authors listed, have made substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
This work was supported by the Office of Naval Research.
Adam, K. C., Mance, I., Fukuda, K., and Vogel, E. K. (2015). The contribution of attentional lapses to individual differences in visual working memory capacity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 1601–1616. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00811
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Amer, T., Campbell, K. L., and Hasher, L. (2016). Cognitive control as a double-edged sword. Trends Cogn Sci. 20, 905–915. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.10.002
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., and Starkes, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 8, 6–16. doi: 10.1037/1076-898x.8.1.6
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bertollo, M., Bortoli, L., Gramaccioni, G., Hanin, Y., Comani, S., and Robazza, C. (2013). Behavioural and psychophysiological correlates of athletic performance: a test of the multi-action plan model. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeed. 38, 91–99. doi: 10.1007/s10484-013-9211-z
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bertollo, M., di Fronso, S., Conforto, S., Schmid, M., Bortoli, L., Comani, S., et al. (2016). Proficient brain for optimal performance: the MAP model perspective. PeerJ. 4:e2082. doi: 10.7717/peerj.2082
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bertollo, M., di Fronso, S., Filho, E., Lamberti, V., Ripari, P., Reis, V. M., et al. (2015). To focus or not to focus: is attention on the core components of action beneficial for cycling performance?. Sport Psychol. 29, 110–119. doi: 10.1123/tsp.2014-0046
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bortoli, L., Bertollo, M., Hanin, Y., and Robazza, C. (2012). Striving for excellence: A multi-action plan intervention model for shooters. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 13, 693–701. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2012.04.006
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bruya, B. (2010). Effortless Attention: A New Perspective in the Cognitive Science of Attention and Action. Boston, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Chang, A., Chen, C. C., Li, H. H., and Li, C. S. (2015). Perigenual anterior cingulate event-related potential precedes stop signal errors. Neuroimage 111, 179–185. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.017
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Critchley, H. D., and Harrison, N. A. (2013). Visceral influences on brain and behavior. Neuron 77, 624–638. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.008
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Critchley, H. D., Mathias, C. J., Josephs, O., O'Doherty, J., Zanini, S., Dewar, B.-K., et al. (2003). Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain. 126, 2139–2152. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg216
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
de Manzano, O., Theorell, T., Harmat, L., and Ullén, F. (2010). The Psychophysiology of flow during piano playing. Emotion 10, 301–311. doi: 10.1037/a0018432
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ding, X., Tang, Y. Y., Deng, Y., Tang, R., and Posner, M. I. (2014b). Mood and personality predict improvement in creativity due to meditation training. Learn. Individ. Differ. 37, 217–221. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.11.019
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ding, X., Tang, Y. Y., Tang, R., and Posner, M. I. (2014a). Improving creativity performance by short-term meditation. Behav. Brain Funct. 10:9. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-10-9
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ekman, M., Fiebach, C. J., Melzer, C., Tittgemeyer, M., and Derrfuss, J. (2016). Different roles of direct and indirect frontoparietal pathways for individual working memory capacity. J. Neurosci. 36, 2894–2903. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1376-14.2016
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Fan, Y., Tang, Y. Y., Tang, R., and Posner, M. I. (2014). Short term integrative meditation improves resting alpha activity and Stroop performance. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeed. 39, 213–217. doi: 10.1007/s10484-014-9258-5
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Fan, Y., Tang, Y. Y., Tang, R., and Posner, M. I. (2015). Time course of conflict processing modulated by brief meditation training. Front. Psychol. 6:911 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00911
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Jacobs, K. A. (2014). Flow State in Dancers: Autonomic Regulation During Performance. M.S. Thesis, California State University, Northridge.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Google Scholar
Keller, J., Bless, H., Blomann, F., and Kleinbohl, D. (2011). Physiological aspects of flow experiences: skills-demand-compatibility effects on heart rate variability and salivary control. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47, 849–852. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.02.004
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., and Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 163–169. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Nissim, N. R., O'Shea, A. M., Bryant, V., Porges, E. C., Cohen, R., and Woods, A. J. (2017). Frontal structural neural correlates of working memory performance in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:328. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00328
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Petersen, S. E., and Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 73–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Reuda, M. R., and Tang, Y. Y. (2010). Effortless Attention: A New Perspective in the Cognitive Science of Attention and Action. Boston, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Stillman, C. M., Feldman, H., Wambach, C. G., Howard, J. H. Jr., and Howard, D. V. (2014). Dispositional mindfulness is associated with reduced implicit learning. Conscious. Cogn. 28, 141–150. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.07.002
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Takeuchi, H., Sekiguchi, A., Taki, Y., Yokoyama, S., Yomogida, Y., Komuro, N., et al. (2010). Training of working memory impacts structural connectivity. J. Neurosci. 30, 3297–3303. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4611-09.2010
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tang, Y.-Y. (2017). The Neuroscience of Mindfulness Meditation–How Body-Mind Work Together to Change Behavior? Springer Nature.
Google Scholar
Tang, Y.-Y., Holzel, B. K., and Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 213–225. doi: 10.1038/nrn3916
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tang, Y.-Y., Ma, Y., Fan, Y., Feng, H., Wang, J., Feng, S., et al. (2009). Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is altered by short term meditation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 8865–8870. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904031106
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tang, Y.-Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., et al. (2007). Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 17152–17156. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707678104
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tang, Y.-Y., and Posner, M. I. (2009). Attention training and attention state training. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 222–227. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.009
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tang, Y.-Y., and Posner, M. I. (2014). Training brain networks and states. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 345–350. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.002
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tang, Y.-Y., Rothbart, M. K., and Posner, M. I. (2012). Neural correlates of establishing, maintaining, and switching brain states. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 330–337. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.05.001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tang, Y.-Y., and Tang, R. (2013). Ventral-subgenual anterior cingulate cortex and self-transcendence. Front. Psychol. 4:1000. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01000
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tang, Y.-Y., Tang, R., Jiang, C., and Posner, M. I. (2014). Short-term meditation intervention improves self-regulation and academic performance. J. Child Adolesc. Behav. 2:4. doi: 10.4172/2375-4494.1000154
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tang, Y.-Y., Tang, Y., Tang, R., and Lewis-Peacock, J. (2017). Brief mental training reorganizes large-scale networks. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 11:6. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2017.00006
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Thomson, P., and Jaque, S. V. (2011). Psychophysiological study: ambulatory measures of the ANS in performing artists. Int. Symp. Perform. Sci. 1, 149–154.
Keywords: mind-body interaction, optimal performance, mindfulness meditation, integrative body–mind training (IBMT), anterior cingulate cortex, striatum, central nervous system (CNS), autonomic nervous system (ANS)
Citation: Tang Y-Y and Bruya B (2017) Mechanisms of Mind-Body Interaction and Optimal Performance. Front. Psychol. 8:647. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00647
Received: 19 February 2017; Accepted: 11 April 2017;
Published: 09 May 2017.
Edited by:
Reviewed by:
Copyright © 2017 Tang and Bruya. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Yi-Yuan Tang, eWl5dWFuYmFsYW5jZUBnbWFpbC5jb20=
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.